Sensory Modulation: A Review of the Literature Print E-mail

Authors: Julia Wilbarger, MS, OTR and Tracy Murnan Stackhouse, OTR

The concept of a disruption in sensory modulation (SMD) has emerged recently as a "hot-topic" within the field of occupational therapy. This was evident at the recent AOTA Annual Conference in Baltimore (April, 1998), where the Sensory Integration Special Interest Section, currently chaired by Jane Koomar, devoted a full day to an institute on sensory modulation. Several OT researchers, some of whom presented at the AOTA Institute, have published or are in the process of publishing new studies related to disruption in sensory modulation in a variety of clinical and normative samples. Additionally, there were several short course and paper topics focusing on this area of theory and practice.

Due to the increasing interest in sensory modulation, it is increasingly important for OT practitioners, researchers, and educators to have a common way to discuss and disseminate information relevant to the area. However, there is limited information written about sensory modulation. Further, referring to this cluster of problems as a disorder may be premature. Our understanding of the nature of this phenomenon is in the very early stages. It has not been identified as having universal and persistent features that are distinct from other established disorders. To that end, it may be more appropriate at this time to describe SMD as a disruption in processing rather than trying to define it as a disorder. (Note: We will use the term disruption but will refer to disorder in our historical review to maintain the authors’ terminology)

Within the field of occupational therapy, current theorists rely upon the core writings of Dr. A. Jean Ayres. Her theory of sensory integration provides the inceptive principles upon which later theorists have built. However, Ayres’ descriptions of the modulation of sensory input are only the beginning. Over the last three decades, the concept of sensory modulation has been informed by the clinical practice of occupational therapists using a sensory integrative frame of reference. This field has expanded the application of occupational therapy intervention to a variety of clinical populations -- each providing insight into sensory processing phenomenon and how disruptions of sensory processing can lead to a variety of problems with carrying out "the skills for the job of living".

The concepts supporting sensory modulation also have evolved in accordance with new findings from neuroscience. As 1990 was deemed the "Decade of the Brain," many changes in thinking have occurred over the course of the last 30 years that need to be incorporated into any theory of brain-behavior relationships. Most prominently, the notion of a hierarchically organized brain has been replaced by a heterarchical, parallel, distributed processing model of brain structure and function. Theoretical models derived from less complex brain system representations do not offer a current conceptualization of what has been discovered about brain functioning. This coupled with the brains’ amazing ability to develop based on experience, whether that be initial development, or reorganization following damage, provides increasing interest in OT theory and intervention. These changes in neuroscience require an updating of our theories to incorporate the new findings in order to facilitate acceptance of our unique perspective on central nervous system functioning as it relates to human occupation.

The following is a brief literature review of the concept of sensory modulation within the field of occupational therapy. This review begins with the seminal work of Ayres and traces the contributions of many outstanding occupational therapy researchers and theoreticians over the years in operationalizing the concept of sensory modulation.

Ayres on modulation

Ayres defined sensory integration as, "the neurological process that organizes sensation from one’s own body and from the environment and makes it possible to use the body effectively within the environment. The spatial and temporal aspects of inputs from different sensory modalities are interpreted, associated, and unified" (Ayres, p. 11, 1989). Over the course of her career, Ayres conceptualized a number of disorders related to disrupted sensory processing. She empirically identified several clusters of behaviors related to specific patterns of test scores and observations. The interpretation of these patterns was based on Ayres’ theory of sensory integration. At the time, Ayres referred to two broad categories of sensory processing (discriminative and protective).

In Ayres’ view, sensory discrimination leads to perception, conceptualization, and action. Ayres’ research delineated the role of discriminative sensory processing in the development of functions such as body scheme, smooth accurate movement, bilateral skill, visual perception and praxis.

In terms of modulation, Ayres discussed the balance of both inhibitory and excitatory influences on the brain. According to Ayres, "the combination of facilitatory and inhibitory messages produces modulation which is the nervous system’s process of self organization" (1979, p. 44-45). "Modulation is the process of increasing or reducing neural activity to keep that activity in harmony with all the other functions of the nervous system" (Ayres, 1979, p. 70). Ayres defined modulation as the "brain’s regulation of it’s own activity" ( 1979, p. 182). It involves facilitating some neural messages to produce an enhanced response and inhibiting other messages to reduce activity. She highlighted the role of the vestibular system in modulating the activity of the other systems. The central connections of the vestibular system place it in a key position to modulate activity in the brain stem, cerebellum, and related ascending pathways.

Over the course of rigorous theory development, Ayres elegantly ascertained several clusters of sensory processing phenomenon and behavioral symptoms. One cluster that emerged from Ayres’ factor analytic studies included tactile defensiveness and hyperactivity. Ayres introduced the concept of tactile defensiveness as avoiding or negative reactions to non-noxious tactile stimuli (Ayres, 1964). She conceptualized tactile defensiveness as an imbalance between discriminative and protective sensory processing. While the concept of tactile defensiveness received the bulk of the attention, Ayres also suggested that hypersensitivities might be observed in other sensory modalities. Ayres is also responsible for coining the terms gravitational and postural insecurity.

Based on a 1980 study of children with autism, Ayres and Tickle suggested that "children who register sensory input but failed to modulate it responded better to therapy than those who were hypo-responsive or failed to orient to sensory input" (p. 375, 1980). They described this failure to orient as inadequate registration of incoming sensory stimuli. They further proposed that SI treatment, "was more effective in modulating sensory input than in helping the brain to register or orient to it" (Ayres, & Tickle, p. 381, 1980).

A central tenant of Ayres conceptualization of sensory integration was the premise of the brain’s ability to regulate its own activity. A classic SI approach utilizes this principle in concert with other guiding principles which include the idea that the treatment must be child-directed, with the therapist guiding the just right challenge in order to facilitate adaptive responses. These core principles guide clinical reasoning and continued theory building in the area of sensory integration.

Knickerbocker

In her 1980 book, A Holistic Approach to Learning Disorders, Barbara Knickerbocker expanded Ayres’ concept of tactile defensiveness to a broader category of sensory defensiveness. Knickerbocker placed sensory defensiveness on a continuum with sensory dormancy. She described sensory dormancy as, "too much…inhibition of incoming sensory stimuli" (p. 32). She continued Ayres’ conceptualization of defensiveness as an imbalance of inhibitory and excitatory forces. In Knickerbocker’s view, too little inhibition resulted in sensory defensiveness and too much inhibition resulted in sensory dormancy.

Dunn and Fisher

In a pair of articles, Ann Fisher and Winnie Dunn (1983; Dunn & Fisher, 1983) proposed a concept that influenced thinking regarding SMD for many years. They proposed that tactile defensiveness and registration problems were the opposing ends of a single continuum. They cite Ayres for suggesting the notion of registration as an emerging, important clinical concept. This was based on the work of Pribram and colleagues (Pribram & McGinnis, 1975). Dunn & Fisher concretized the notion of registration and dichotomously contrasted it with defensiveness. This continuum notion became the central tenant of thinking about sensory modulation for the next decade.

Cermak

The state of knowledge regarding modulation of sensation was in its infancy during the 1980’s. Toward the end of this period, Sharon Cermak wrote a two part series for the AOTA (American Occupational Therapy Association) Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, on the role of sensory integration in understanding and treating attention deficits (Cermak, 1988). In pediatric occupational therapy, the SIS Newsletters have traditionally provided an excellent forum for fostering clinical reasoning and promoting communication between research and practice. This article served both of these purposes, and continues to provide an important summative backdrop for current thinking about sensory modulation. In this set of articles, Cermak summarized tenets of sensory modulation from within the field of OT as well as bridging the gap of knowledge regarding the relevant issues from outside of the field of OT.

Cermak provided a valuable summary of perspectives on arousal and attention from researchers outside of the field of OT. She included in this paper a presentation of a working model of the multiple continuums that play a role in arousal and attention mechanisms. Each offered separate linear continuums of various aspects of processing related to attention and arousal as follows:

Kinsbourne’s (1987) continuum opposes underfocused attention and hyperactive behavior with overfocused attention and overanxious behavior.
Zentall and Zentall’s (1983) arousal continuum, comprising low to high CNS arousal in a linear relationship.
Porges’ (1976) ANS continuum, with polar relations between parasympathetic and sympathetic divisions.
Each of these continua portray a linear relationship. What is interesting about this theory period is that each of the versions discuss different aspects of processing, and yet theorizing within the field of OT continued to lump these together into one broad linear continuum. The OT models attempted to capture the essence of each of the separate continua but in so doing reduced the complexity of neural function into a model that while simple, was inaccurate. This served an initial purpose of providing a framework for thinking about SMD. However, as Cermak’s review highlights, any simple, linear conceptualization while appealing, is misleading.

Fisher, Murray and Bundy

As a new decade unfolded, so did thinking about SMD. Fisher, Murray and Bundy, in their 1991 text, Sensory Integration Theory and Practice, included SMD in a revised version of the classic Ayres sensory integration flow chart. In their description of disorders in sensory modulation, Fisher, Murray and Bundy, included aversive responses, gravitational insecurity, sensory defensiveness and tactile defensiveness. The major observable behavioral signs of SMD-- avoidance, distractibility and increased activity level--were suggested as the end products of such a disruption (Fisher, Murray and Bundy, 1990).

Lane and Royeen

Royeen (1989) elaborated on a continuum model where sensory defensiveness and sensory dormancy, together, can be considered sensory modulation disorders and are on opposing ends of a sensory responsivity/registration continuum with over orientation at one end and a failure to orient on the other end. Royeen further noted that this continuum might in fact be circular with sensory dormancy and defensiveness as potentially adjacent functions.

Royeen and Lane (1991) advanced the notion of a midrange between the hyper and hypo responsive ends of the linear continuums. They described individuals who had difficulty achieving mid-range responses on the defensiveness-dormancy continuum. Royeen and Lane describe the concept of deficiencies in the modulation of sensation as being key to explaining sensory defensiveness and sensory dormancy. This was in contrast to simply considering poor inhibition, which traditionally had been the mechanism used to explain SMD in previous OT models.

Koomar and Bundy

Koomar and Bundy (1991) state, "when an individual overresponds, underresponds or fluctuates in response to sensory input in a manner disproportional to that input, we say that the individual has a sensory modulation disorder" (p. 268, 1991). They also describe a complex condition, which paradoxical to a continuum model, allows for an individual to have problems with both discrimination and modulation of sensation. Their chapter also provided a nice overview of treatment approaches for tactile defensiveness, gravitational insecurity, aversive responding to movement, and poor registration.

Wilbarger & Wilbarger

Wilbarger & Wilbarger (1991) promote the concept of sensory defensiveness as consistent with normal defensive responses. They propose that sensory defensiveness is on a continuum of approach and avoidance behaviors. This is in direct contrast to the sensory defensiveness/poor registration continuum promoted by other theorists. The Wilbargers’ suggest that sensory defensiveness is one of a subset of sensory modulation disruptions.

Kimball

Kimball (1993) describes sensory modulation problems in terms of the arousal state created due to the influences of sensation. "Persons who have sensory system modulation problems have more changeable arousal or reaction levels than normal. This results in problems with adaptive responses because there systems lack stability" (p 96-97). Kimball related these behavioral phenomenon to the classic inverted U shaped curve (Hebb, 1949), highlighting an important concept that "sensory system modulation fluctuates within a range of normal,"(p. 98). In this conceptualization, performance and adaptive capacity are poor at both low and high level of arousal. In the middle ranges of arousal, performance and adaptation are optimal. Additionally, Kimball reiterated the notion that a single sensory system may not be isolable, "sensory systems do not function independently. Arousal in several systems can combine" (p. 97, 1993).


Importantly, Kimball also introduced the concept of shut down which she described as a protective mechanism against severe overload. She illustrated the notion of a non-linear continuum by describing individuals at the extremes of behavior i.e., "some children even react in a dangerous way and go from overarousal to physiological shut down."

Parham and Mailloux

Parham and Mailloux (1996) define sensory modulation as "a tendency to generate responses that are appropriately graded in relation to incoming sensory stimuli rather than under reacting or overreacting to them." They support the views expressed by Cermak (1988) and Royeen (1989) that modulation disorders are represented by a continuum from registration problems to sensory defensiveness. They describe dysfunction as either fluctuations or a tendency to function at one extreme or the other. They further describe sensory registration problems as a failure to attend to or register relevant environmental stimuli. Parham and Mailloux note that registration problems are often seen in children with autism and developmental delays. Usually more than one sensory system is involved, but there are inconsistencies such as ignoring "relevant stimuli while overfocusing on irrelevant stimuli" (p. 324), or "to lack sensory registration in some situations but react with extreme sensory defensiveness in others" (p. 324). Children lacking appropriate registration may engage in intense or dangerous behaviors. They also describe a situation where lack of registration creates a lack of inner drive to engage in typical childhood occupations and is detrimental to long term development.

Empirical work currently shaping the concept of SMD

At present, there are a number of researchers endeavoring to expand our knowledge regarding disruptions in sensory processing.

Winnie Dunn and her colleagues have been developing a normed parent questionnaire, the Sensory Profile (Dunn & Westman, 1997). An analysis of the responses made by parents of typical children on the 125 items revealed nine factors related to sensory integration some of which reflect sensory modulation and reactiveness (Dunn & Brown, 1997). These factors include sensory seeking, emotional reactivity, low endurance/tone, oral sensitivity, inattention/distractibility, poor registration, sensory sensitivity, sedentary, and fine motor/perceptual. Dunn further has suggested that sensory modulation needs to be considered on the multiple dimension of a person’s threshold for sensory events and responsiveness to sensation (Dunn, 1997).

Several researchers have been working to understand further the nature of sensory defensiveness. This collection of work is beginning to verify long held clinical opinions that disruptions in sensory modulation are identifiable phenomenon that effect the daily lives of individuals. Moya Kinneally and her colleagues have been studying the phenomenology of sensory defensiveness in adults. They have conducted several studies on the association of sensory defensiveness and anxiety, depression, locus of control orientation, and perception of pain in typical adults. They have also explored treatment of sensory defensiveness using a sensory diet model, in the tradition of the Wilbarger approach. Grace Baranek and her colleagues have conducted a series of studies (Baranek & Berkson, 1994; Baranek, Foster & Berkson, 1997) to understand the role of sensory defensiveness in individuals with developmental disabilities and autism. Between 3-30 % of their sample of individuals with developmental disabilities had sensory defensiveness. She also provides supporting evidence for sensory defensiveness as a unifying construct, and within this, her evidence suggests that there may be smaller subsets of modality specific disruptions, such as auditory sensitivity. These studies are adding to the body of understanding that sensory defensiveness may be a unique disruption of sensory modulation. As such, it appears to be a legitimate phenomenon and not necessarily a by-product of another disorder.

Recent work by Lucy Miller and colleagues at The Children’s Hospital in Denver has begun to quantify the notion of sensory modulation through the use of psychophysiological measurement of sensory reactivity. In a series of articles (Miller & McIntosh, 1998; Miller et al, in press), Miller has identified patterns of unusual sensory reactivity in a variety of groups. These studies are providing some interesting insights that support continued efforts to conceptualize and quantify disruptions in sensory modulation.

Conclusions and Future Directions

Just as the early work of Ayres included syndrome validation, the on-going research of Dunn, Kinnealey, Baranek, Miller, and others is beginning the process of validating the concept of SMD.

The review presented here summarizes the primary notions related to the concept of sensory modulation within the field of occupational therapy. The early work of Ayres began this endeavor with her findings of the noteworthy cluster of tactile defensiveness and hyperactivity. This was followed by the multiple discussions of how a single continuum might describe the clinical presentation of poor modulation. However, recent work is beginning to question this limited concept. Dunn has suggested that there are probably multiple, interacting continua. Clearly, the brain is a complex heterarchy. As such, any model trying to solidify a brain-behavior relationship must reflect that complexity. Therefore, the future of sensory modulation will probably include complex models comprised of dynamic, interactive systems.

Two important concepts of modulation that have been reviewed in this article relate to the ideas of sensory reactivity and optimal arousal. At this point in the OT literature, there is not a model of sensory modulation that incorporates both of these concepts in concert. This important step needs to be taken in order to move the theory of SMD forward.

In our review of the literature, we have been unable to discover a satisfying definition of sensory modulation. We therefore propose the following conceptualization of sensory modulation as useful in considering the complexity of processing and behavior involved.

Sensory modulation is the intake of sensation via typical sensory processing mechanisms such that the degree, intensity and quality of response is graded to match environmental demand and so that a range of optimal performance/adaptation is maintained.

While the linear continuum models that have been the dominant means of conceptualizing SMD served an important purpose, it is apparent that the nervous system mechanisms that support modulation are many, varied and complex. As such, a new model that captures the dynamic, interacting networks is required. Moreover, within the field of occupational therapy, research regarding the occupational nature of SMD should be initiated.

About the Authors

Tracy Stackhouse, OTR, received her degree in OT from Colorado State University. She is SIPT certified and NDT trained. She worked for nearly a decade as the clinical specialist in sensory integration at the Children’s Hospital in Denver, and as the OT for the Fragile X Syndrome Treatment Center. She is currently working on her Ph.D. in developmental cognitive neuroscience with Dr. Bruce Pennington at the University of Denver.

Tracy can be contacted through e-mail at: This e-mail address is being protected from spam bots, you need JavaScript enabled to view it

Julia Wilbarger, MS, OTR, received a B.S. in physiological psychology from the University of California, Berkeley and a M.S. in Occupational Therapy from Boston University. She has worked as an occupational therapist in early intervention with high risk and developmentally delayed infants and toddlers, in private practice and the public schools. Julia was the clinical specialist for the sensory integration team at the Children’s Hospital in Denver for several years. She is the co-founder of Avanti, a sensory integrative summer camp and was the administrative director of that project for several years. Julia lectures nationally and internationally on sensory defensiveness and has served on the Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Standing Committee of the AOTA. She is currently in a doctoral program at the University of Denver, Colorado.

Julia can be contacted through e-mail at: This e-mail address is being protected from spam bots, you need JavaScript enabled to view it

References

Ayres, A.J. (1 964). Tactile functions: The relation to hyperactive and perceptual motor behavior. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20, 288-292.

Ayres, A.J. (1 972). Sensory Integration and Learning Disorders. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Ayres, A.J. (1 979). Sensory Integration and the Child. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Ayres, A.J., & Tickle, L.S. (1 980). Hyperresponsivity to touch and vestibular stimuli as a predictor of positive response to sensory integration procedures by autistic children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 31, 444-453.

Ayres, A.J. (1 989). Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests. Los Angeles, Western Psychological Services.

Baranek, G.T., & Berkson, G. (1 994). Tactile defensiveness in children with developmental disabilities: Responsiveness and habituation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 457-471.

Baranek, G.T., Foster, L.G., & Berkson, G. (1997). Tactile Defensiveness and Stereotyped Behavior. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51(2), 91-95.

Cermak, S. (1988). The relationship between attention and sensory Integration Dysfunction (Part 1), Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 11, 1-4. (Published by AOTA, Bethesda, MD)

DeGangi, G., & Greenspan, S. (1988). The Development of sensory functioning in infants. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 8(3), 21-33.

Dunn, W., & Westman, K. (1 997). The Sensory Profile: The Performance of a National Sample of Children without Disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51(l), 25-34.

Dunn, W., & Brown, C, (1 997). Factor analysis on the Sensory Profile from a national sample of children without disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 51, 490-495.

Dunn, W. (1997). The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: A conceptual model. Infants and Young Children, 9 (4), 23-35.

Dunn, W., & Fisher, A. (1983). Sensory registration, autism and tactile defensiveness. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 6 (2), 3-4. (Published by AOTA, Bethesda, MD)

Fisher, A., Murray, E., & Bundy, A. (1991). Sensory Integration Theory and Practice. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis Company.

Fisher, A., & Dunn, W. (1983). Tactile defensiveness: Historical perspectives, new research--a theory grows. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 6 (2), 3-4. (Published by AOTA, Bethesda, MD)

Gilfoyle, E., Grady, A.P., & Moore, J.C. (I 992). Children Adapt (2 ed.). Thorofare, New Jersey: Charles B. Slack Inc.

Greenough, W.T., & Alcantara, A.A. (1993). The Roles of experience in different developmental information stage processes. Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Hebb, D. (1 949). Organization of behavior. - A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.

Kandel, E., Schwartz, J., & Jessel, T. (1991). Principles of Neuroscience (Vol.3). Norwalk: Appleton & Lange

Kimball, J. (1993). Sensory Integration Frame of Reference. In P. Kramer & J. Hinojosa (Eds.), Frames of Reference in Pediatric Occupational Therapy Philadelphia: Williams & Wilkins.

Kinnealey, M. (1973). Aversive and non-aversive responses to sensory stimulation in mentally retarded children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 27(8).

Kinnealey, M., Oliver, B., Pfeiffer, B., Fuiek, M., & Gebert, M. (1998) Sensory Defensiveness in Adults. Paper presented at the American Occupational Therapy Association Annual Conference, April, 1998. Baltimore.

Kinsbourne, M. (1987). Specific learning disabilities and attention deficit with hyperactivity. In M. Gottlieb, J. Williams, P. Zinkus (eds). Textbook of developmental pediatrics. NY: Plenum Press.

Lane, S. & Royeen, C. (1991). Tactile processing and sensory defensiveness. In Fisher, Murray, & Bundy (eds.), Sensory Integration Theory and Practice. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis Company.

Knickerbocker, B. (I 980). A Holistic Approach to Learning Disabilities Thorofare, NJ: Charles B. Slack.

LeDoux, J. E. (1 994). Emotion, Memory and the Brain. Scientific American, 50-57. Mattingly, C., & Fleming, M. H. (I 994). Clinical Reasoning: Forms of inquiry in a therapeutic process. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.

McGinnis, D., & Pribram, K. (1980). The neuropsychology of attention emotional and motivational controls. In E. Wittrack (Ed.), The Brain and Psychology: Academic Press.

Miller, L., & Mcintosh, D. (1 998). The diagnosis, treatment and etiology of sensory modulation disorder. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Newsletter, 21 (1), 1-3.

Miller, L., McIntosh, D., McGrath, J., Shyu, V., Lampe, M., Taylor, A., Tassone, F., Neitzel, K., Stackhouse, T., & Hagerman, R. (1998), Electrodermal Responses to Sensory Stimuli in Individuals with Fragile X Syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics (in press).

Parham, L.D., & Mailloux, Z. (1996). Sensory Integration and Children with Learning Disabilities. In P.N. Pratt & A.S. Allen (Eds.), Occupational Therapy in Children (pp. 307-355). St. Louis: Mosby.

Pennington, B. F., & Welsh, M. (1 996). Neuropsychology and Developmental Psychopathology. In C.a. Cohen (Ed.), Manual of Developmental Psychopathology, Vol, I (pp. 254-290).

Porges, S. (1976). Peripheral and neurochemical parallels of psychopathology: A psychophysiological model relating autonomic imbalance to hyperactivity, pschyopatholgy and autism. In Advances in Child development and Behavior (Vol 11). NY: Academic Press.

Pribram, K. (I 994). Brain and Perception: Holonomy and structure in figural processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum.

Pribram, K., & McGinnis, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention. Psychology Review, 82, 116-149.

Royeen, C. (1989). Commentary of "Tactile functions in learning disabled and normal children: Reliability and validity considerations". Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 9, 16-23.

Tickle-Degnen, L. (1988). Perspective on the status of sensory integration theory. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 42, 427-433.

Wilbarger, P. & Wilbarger, J. (1991) Sensory Defensiveness in Children 2-12: An Intervention Guide. Avanti Educational Programs, Denver.

Zentall, S., & Zentall, T. (1983). Optimal stimulation: a model of disordered activity and performance in normal and deviant children. Psych Bulletin, 94, 466-471.

Zuckerman, M. (1993). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Revised September 12, 1998

Copyright 1998 - The KID Foundation, 1901 West Littleton Blvd., Littleton, CO 80120


 

Last Updated ( Saturday, 17 June 2006 )
 
Next >

Attendee Comments

Comment from the January 2006 conference:
A Nonlinear Dynamics Approach to Sensory Modulation

Tina Champagne blew me away! She made me proud to be an OT and inspired to get back into psych OT! - Hollie Marron, OTR/L